240 likes | 255 Views
Data, methods and units of analysis. PhD Research Design Course Tobias Hagmann , Roskilde University thagmann@ruc.dk January 30, 2018. Objectives of this session. Think through four key questions that are vital for your doctoral project and its research design, namely:
E N D
Data, methods and units of analysis PhD Research Design Course Tobias Hagmann, Roskilde University thagmann@ruc.dk January 30, 2018
Objectives of this session Think through four key questions that are vital for your doctoral project and its research design, namely: • What is my unit and level of analysis? • What’s my data like and where can I find it? • What is my thesis (really) about? • How do I do it?
Unit and level of analysis -> unit of analysis and level of analysisarecrucial for a good research design 1) unit of analysis: • whatexactly do I study? • sometimes referred to as ‘variables’ (dependent/independent) • causal relations between units of analysis (ex: relation between X and Y) • don’tconfuseconcepts with units of analysis (although overlap) 2) levelof analysis: • geographical perimeter, conceptualboundary, level of aggregation • knowing the boundaries of one’s research topic (and unit of analysis), but alsoitsrelevance for the broader population of units
Don’tbemislead by yourdiscipline • The unit of analysis is in the eye of the beholder. (‘Le point de vue créel’objet’, Bachelard). • Academic disciplines have theirfavorite units of analysis(whichoftenbecame ‘normalized’). • Knowledge is compartementalized as differentdisciplines ‘colonize’ particular units of analysis, eventhoughtheyrefer to same or similarempiricalrealities and social phenomena. • Be conscience of howyousituateyourselfwithinyourdiscipline (and field of study).
Discuss with yourneighbour • What is mymain unit of analysis? • What is mymainlevel of analysis? • How is what I do different from whatothersaredoing, have done?
Empirical reality is complex • Social scienciststry to find, at times impose uniform, neatlydelineatedrationalities, logics, and causalities, but empirical reality and data areoften • ambiguous and contradictory • incomplete and messy • the result of preselection and previous definitions • difficult, at times impossible to access • Do not assumethatyourtheory is more complicatedthanyourempiricalmaterial! • The betteryourknowyour ‘empirics’, your ‘data’, the betteryourthesis. • Good PhDthesestypically provide original (primary) empiricalmaterial.
Locatingprimary data • the betterweknowwhereour data ‘sits’ (or ‘lives’), the higher the chance thatwecanaccess and document it • our ‘data’ exists in multiple forms: • primary versus secondary • visible and invisible • individual and collective • expert versus popular • embodied and disembodied • material and symbolic • locating data is a continuos ‘try and error’ process (need to remain open throughout research process)
Actors and data • social actors (= human beings) produce and store information or ‘data’ in different forms: • throughtheireverydayexperiences (practices) • through learning and exchanging with otherhumans (knowledge) • by forming moral judgementsabout the world (moral conceptions) • by codifying data themselves (databases, archives) • but alsoforget and ‘cloud’ information includingtheirownexperiences(temporality of remembered events) => all of the above is data wecanuse to explain social phenomena
Sites and data • data and actorsarelinked to particular sites, places and spaces, whichneed to beidentified • a research site has its • own potential and limitation for data collection(ex. marketplace vs. private home) • is differentiated in terms of access (public, semi-public, ‘specialized’, private) • has itsownspatiality and temporality • the more youbecomeassociated with a site, the better chances for data collection (but alsorisk of being ‘sucked in’)
Application and discussion Take some 10 minutes and produce a tableindicating • maindifferent types of data thatinformsyourthesis • actors or institutions thatpossess or producethis data • sites whereyoucangather (or have gathered) this data Try to be as specific as possible!
Thinking of yourthesis as a ‘case’ • Not case study research design, but: doctoral research object as a case thatrelates to a broader population of cases • Forces us to thinkthrough relations between: • the specific and the general • the concrete and the abstract • the unique and the generic • Identifydifferentlevels of abstractionthatarealwasys present in a (doctoral) research project
Key points Flyvbjerg (2006) • Misunderstanding 1: General, theoretical (context-independent) knowledge is more valuable than concrete, practical (context-dependent) knowledge. • Misunderstanding 2: One cannot generalize on the basis of an individual case; therefore, the case study cannot contribute to scientific development. • Misunderstanding 3: The case study is most useful for generating hypotheses; that is, in the first stage of a total research process, whereas other methods are moresuitablefor hypotheses testing and theory building. • Misunderstanding 4: The case study contains a bias toward verification, that is, a tendency to confirm the researcher’s preconceived notions. • Misunderstanding 5: It is often difficult to summarize and develop general propositions and theories on the basis of specific case studies.
The ‘bible’ of case study research design • Yin, R. K. (2003). Case Study Research: Design and Methods. London and New Delhi, SAGE Publications.
Discussion in class • What is your case? • What does it contribute to your discipline or field? • What is particular about your case (what makes it special?)? • What are the limits of generalizability of your case? • How does your case connect to broader theories and theory development? • Who will care about your case (which stakeholders)?
Methodologicalchoices • In principlechoice of methods is guided by research question and objective. • In reality it is often the result of a mixture of: • pragmatism • personalpreference • ‘howothers have done it’ • Keep in mind: methodsareoften the most ‘practical’ and ‘applied’ skillthatyouuse for yourthesis. Choosethemwisely. Theywillco-determineyour future academicprofile.
Data access and the researcher • Research design and field research data collectionneed to takeintoaccount the researcher’spositionality, in particulargender, age, nationality, religion, class etc. • Choose a data collectionstrategythatmaximisesyour ‘identity’ and positionality(and identifyyour limits explicitly). • Considerpersonalskills, preferences and interests in designing data collectionstrategy. • Use ‘positionality’ as a comparativeadvantage: what kind of data canyouaccess and analyzethatnobodyelsecan?
Differentmethods, mixingmethods • qualitative vs. quantitative research traditions (Goertz & Mahoney, 2012) • differentlogics of inference: typically large-N cross-case analysis vs. small N or in-case analysis • in reality every social phenomenoncanbebothqualified and quantified (and data can at times betransformed from one to another) • short-sighted ‘hatred’ between the two research traditions • different types of mixed methodsapproaches (seeCreswell, 2013) • ‘complementary social science’ (Blok & Pedersen, 2014)
Discussion in class • What is your positionality? • Which kind of data, actors and sites can you access easily – which ones require extra effort – and which ones are off limit? • How will ‘informants’ perceive you and your research topic? • How do your personal skills, preferences and interests inform your data collection strategy? • What kind of data can you produce that nobody else can? • What motivates your choice of method?