340 likes | 354 Views
Risk perception and risk taking: Psychological perspectives. J. Richard Eiser Centre for Research in Social Attitudes Department of Psychology University of Sheffield. Risk and probability: Annual risk of dying from. Smoking 10 cigarettes a day 1 in 200 Influenza 1 in 5000
E N D
Risk perception and risk taking:Psychological perspectives. J. Richard Eiser Centre for Research in Social Attitudes Department of Psychology University of Sheffield
Risk and probability:Annual risk of dying from... • Smoking 10 cigarettes a day 1 in 200 • Influenza 1 in 5000 • Road accident 1 in 8000 • Accident at home 1 in 26000 • Accident at work 1 in 43500 • Homicide 1 in 100000 • Railway accident 1 in 500000
Estimating probability. • Some risks are underestimated, some are overestimated, and some are unknown. • Statistical reasoning is difficult, so people rely on ‘rules of thumb’, e.g. frequency of news reports, personal experience. • Personal experience can be unrepresentative. • People often don’t see population statistics as personally applicable.
Where do risks come from? • Risk involves uncertainty about the likelihood of events and the value of their consequences. • Risk arises from interactions between people and their social and physical environment. • Risk depends not only on physical conditions but also on human actions and decisions.
Risk and uncertainty Risky decisions involve uncertainty about: • What will happen (likelihood); • How good or bad the consequences will be (value). • Risk controversies can reflect divergent values as well as disagreements over facts.
Key psychological processes • Attitudes – what do we want, like or fear? • Decisions – how do we choose what to do? • Learning – how do we come by our attitudes and beliefs? • Social influence – how do other people influence our attitudes, beliefs and behaviour?
Attitudes Communicators, advertisers, etc. attempt to change people’s attitudes by: • Changing perceived likelihood of consequences; • Associating positive or negative value with objects or activities. • Such associations may be emotional rather than factual.
Decision-making under uncertainty Danger Cautious criterion ? Risky criterion Safety
Learning • We tend to repeat actions, or stick with beliefs, that have brought us success or pleasure in the past (‘reinforcement’). • We tend to avoid activities and objects that have brought us failure or displeasure. • Hence, we tend to ‘over-sample’ more familiar (trusted) activities and objects. • Expectancies of good or bad outcomes may be confirmed or contradicted by experience.
Feedback from experience is typically selective • Approach behaviour can produce informative feedback whereas avoidance behaviour does not. • If you avoid something you believe to be dangerous, you will typically not discover whether your fears were justified. • This means that ‘risk-averse’ fears and prejudices may persist because they aren’t fully tested.
Feedback may be delayed or inconsistent • Risk-taking (incorrect approach behaviour) may also persist because feedback is… • Delayed – e.g. harmful effects of smoking; • Inconsistent – e.g. not all cases of dangerous driving lead to accidents.
The Beanfest Project Collaborators: Russell Fazio Natalie Shook (Ohio State) Tom Stafford Tony Prescott (Sheffield)
Beanfest • Beanfest is a virtual world containing good and bad beans. • Good beans give you energy, bad beans make you lose energy. If you lose too much energy, or never eat, you die. • You have to learn which beans are good and bad, but to do this you have to try them. If you don’t try them, you’ll never find out.
The beans • The beans vary in terms of two dimensions: • Shape, from round to oblong • Number of speckles • Clusters of good and bad beans are located in different parts of the 2-d ‘space’.
region 6 region 6 region 5 region 4 region 4 region 3 region 2 region 2 region 1
Experimental findings • Participants find the task quite difficult. • Good beans are learnt less well than bad beans, some good beans being consistently avoided as though they are bad. • Generalisation occurs to untrained beans on basis of proximity (similarity). • Participants learn to correct positive but not negative ‘prejudices’.
“Points” Experiment:Mean Proportion Correctas a Function of Feedback and ValenceTest Phase
Results of simulations • Bad beans are learnt better than good beans. • Some good beans are consistently avoided as though they are bad. • Such learning generalises to novel beans with similar features. • If connections are ‘seeded’ to produce specific errors, the network corrects positive, but not negative, ‘prejudice’.
Implications for decision-making If decisions are guided by expectancies… • Choices will be risk-averse (for gains) where this yields satisfactory outcomes. • Individuals will experience more positive than negative outcomes. • Unknown objects will be assumed to be more negative than positive.
And in real life.. • If we have sufficient resources and freedom of action, we may avoid anything we expect, rightly or wrongly, to be unsafe. • Most of our experiences will then be happy and we’ll be pretty pleased with ourselves. • But many people don’t have the resources or freedom to control their experiences in this way.
Social influence • Where we lack direct personal experience it often makes sense to imitate, or learn from, others. But which others? • We are more likely to trust our friends. • Our friends will tend to have similar attitudes and experiences to our own. • Hence, social networks will tend to reinforce our existing attitudes. • Media may amplify or attenuate the perceived seriousness of hazards.
Trust • Trust can involve reliance on others both as controllers of risks and as informants about the extent of any risk. • Trust can depend on implicit estimates of the others’ competence, partiality and honesty. • If ‘experts’ are seen as having a vested interest, this may undermine trust.
Why risk perceptions resist change • We (fail to) test our prior beliefs, by looking for confirmation rather than falsification. • We rely on those we like and trust for information, but such people are more likely to share and confirm our prior beliefs. • We all process information selectively, but typically lack insight into how selective we’re being.